ERP = EEG - noise?












1














As I understand it, ERPs can be reliably identified only after the noise (inherent to EEG signals, regardless of the task used to evoke the response) has been largely removed, which is done by averaging across multiple repetitions. The ERP in that case is a recognisable-enough pattern in the EEG signal that it can be reliably pointed out in a graph of the averaged trials, for several electrodes, for instance:



enter image description here



My question: if the averaging method is trustworthy enough - assuming enough-many reptitions - to make noise components cancel each other out while leaving reliable effects still in, then why are other non-zero sections of (ripples in) the EEG signal also not defined as ERPs, with some attached meaning (e.g. response to some cognitive task), since they have, after all, survived the same denoising procedure?



It would make sense to trust the EEG signal to really not reflect any effect when the intercept (y) is zero. However, in most EEG plots this seems to (very conveniently!) only happen at the t=0 point. The signal then becomes non-zero but only part of is acknowledged as an ERP, whereas the rest seems to be considered noise even though the plotted signal has been denoised.










share|improve this question





























    1














    As I understand it, ERPs can be reliably identified only after the noise (inherent to EEG signals, regardless of the task used to evoke the response) has been largely removed, which is done by averaging across multiple repetitions. The ERP in that case is a recognisable-enough pattern in the EEG signal that it can be reliably pointed out in a graph of the averaged trials, for several electrodes, for instance:



    enter image description here



    My question: if the averaging method is trustworthy enough - assuming enough-many reptitions - to make noise components cancel each other out while leaving reliable effects still in, then why are other non-zero sections of (ripples in) the EEG signal also not defined as ERPs, with some attached meaning (e.g. response to some cognitive task), since they have, after all, survived the same denoising procedure?



    It would make sense to trust the EEG signal to really not reflect any effect when the intercept (y) is zero. However, in most EEG plots this seems to (very conveniently!) only happen at the t=0 point. The signal then becomes non-zero but only part of is acknowledged as an ERP, whereas the rest seems to be considered noise even though the plotted signal has been denoised.










    share|improve this question



























      1












      1








      1







      As I understand it, ERPs can be reliably identified only after the noise (inherent to EEG signals, regardless of the task used to evoke the response) has been largely removed, which is done by averaging across multiple repetitions. The ERP in that case is a recognisable-enough pattern in the EEG signal that it can be reliably pointed out in a graph of the averaged trials, for several electrodes, for instance:



      enter image description here



      My question: if the averaging method is trustworthy enough - assuming enough-many reptitions - to make noise components cancel each other out while leaving reliable effects still in, then why are other non-zero sections of (ripples in) the EEG signal also not defined as ERPs, with some attached meaning (e.g. response to some cognitive task), since they have, after all, survived the same denoising procedure?



      It would make sense to trust the EEG signal to really not reflect any effect when the intercept (y) is zero. However, in most EEG plots this seems to (very conveniently!) only happen at the t=0 point. The signal then becomes non-zero but only part of is acknowledged as an ERP, whereas the rest seems to be considered noise even though the plotted signal has been denoised.










      share|improve this question















      As I understand it, ERPs can be reliably identified only after the noise (inherent to EEG signals, regardless of the task used to evoke the response) has been largely removed, which is done by averaging across multiple repetitions. The ERP in that case is a recognisable-enough pattern in the EEG signal that it can be reliably pointed out in a graph of the averaged trials, for several electrodes, for instance:



      enter image description here



      My question: if the averaging method is trustworthy enough - assuming enough-many reptitions - to make noise components cancel each other out while leaving reliable effects still in, then why are other non-zero sections of (ripples in) the EEG signal also not defined as ERPs, with some attached meaning (e.g. response to some cognitive task), since they have, after all, survived the same denoising procedure?



      It would make sense to trust the EEG signal to really not reflect any effect when the intercept (y) is zero. However, in most EEG plots this seems to (very conveniently!) only happen at the t=0 point. The signal then becomes non-zero but only part of is acknowledged as an ERP, whereas the rest seems to be considered noise even though the plotted signal has been denoised.







      eeg






      share|improve this question















      share|improve this question













      share|improve this question




      share|improve this question








      edited Dec 10 at 15:39

























      asked Dec 10 at 14:44









      z8080

      4802412




      4802412






















          1 Answer
          1






          active

          oldest

          votes


















          2














          Averaging does not perfectly remove the noise. Assuming the signal is perfectly correlated across repetitions/sweeps/trials and the noise is independent, than doing from 1 to 2 sweeps will increase the SNR by 3 dB (i.e., decrease the noise by 1/2), Every doubling in the number of sweeps increases the SNR by 3 dB so that after 1024 sweeps the SNR has improved by at most 30 dB. This means that after averaging there is a minimum magnitude required for a peak/notch to be considered statistically reliable. Any ripples with smaller magnitudes are ignored since they are not statistically reliable.






          share|improve this answer





















          • This sounds to me (maybe I'm wrong) like a conflation of statistical significance and effect size. If there is also a cut-off in terms of amplitude, then why isn't it explicitly stated in papers (or is it?)
            – z8080
            Dec 10 at 16:12






          • 1




            @z8080 the first paper finds a peak/notch an assesses whether they are reliable or not. Then future papers ask if changes in the peak/notch are reliable or not. These future papers are not looking to see if there are new features in the waveform.
            – StrongBad
            Dec 10 at 16:20










          • I see - I think that explains the apparent paradox I was asking about. Thanks!
            – z8080
            Dec 10 at 16:36











          Your Answer





          StackExchange.ifUsing("editor", function () {
          return StackExchange.using("mathjaxEditing", function () {
          StackExchange.MarkdownEditor.creationCallbacks.add(function (editor, postfix) {
          StackExchange.mathjaxEditing.prepareWmdForMathJax(editor, postfix, [["$", "$"], ["\\(","\\)"]]);
          });
          });
          }, "mathjax-editing");

          StackExchange.ready(function() {
          var channelOptions = {
          tags: "".split(" "),
          id: "391"
          };
          initTagRenderer("".split(" "), "".split(" "), channelOptions);

          StackExchange.using("externalEditor", function() {
          // Have to fire editor after snippets, if snippets enabled
          if (StackExchange.settings.snippets.snippetsEnabled) {
          StackExchange.using("snippets", function() {
          createEditor();
          });
          }
          else {
          createEditor();
          }
          });

          function createEditor() {
          StackExchange.prepareEditor({
          heartbeatType: 'answer',
          autoActivateHeartbeat: false,
          convertImagesToLinks: false,
          noModals: true,
          showLowRepImageUploadWarning: true,
          reputationToPostImages: null,
          bindNavPrevention: true,
          postfix: "",
          imageUploader: {
          brandingHtml: "Powered by u003ca class="icon-imgur-white" href="https://imgur.com/"u003eu003c/au003e",
          contentPolicyHtml: "User contributions licensed under u003ca href="https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/3.0/"u003ecc by-sa 3.0 with attribution requiredu003c/au003e u003ca href="https://stackoverflow.com/legal/content-policy"u003e(content policy)u003c/au003e",
          allowUrls: true
          },
          noCode: true, onDemand: true,
          discardSelector: ".discard-answer"
          ,immediatelyShowMarkdownHelp:true
          });


          }
          });














          draft saved

          draft discarded


















          StackExchange.ready(
          function () {
          StackExchange.openid.initPostLogin('.new-post-login', 'https%3a%2f%2fpsychology.stackexchange.com%2fquestions%2f21283%2ferp-eeg-noise%23new-answer', 'question_page');
          }
          );

          Post as a guest















          Required, but never shown

























          1 Answer
          1






          active

          oldest

          votes








          1 Answer
          1






          active

          oldest

          votes









          active

          oldest

          votes






          active

          oldest

          votes









          2














          Averaging does not perfectly remove the noise. Assuming the signal is perfectly correlated across repetitions/sweeps/trials and the noise is independent, than doing from 1 to 2 sweeps will increase the SNR by 3 dB (i.e., decrease the noise by 1/2), Every doubling in the number of sweeps increases the SNR by 3 dB so that after 1024 sweeps the SNR has improved by at most 30 dB. This means that after averaging there is a minimum magnitude required for a peak/notch to be considered statistically reliable. Any ripples with smaller magnitudes are ignored since they are not statistically reliable.






          share|improve this answer





















          • This sounds to me (maybe I'm wrong) like a conflation of statistical significance and effect size. If there is also a cut-off in terms of amplitude, then why isn't it explicitly stated in papers (or is it?)
            – z8080
            Dec 10 at 16:12






          • 1




            @z8080 the first paper finds a peak/notch an assesses whether they are reliable or not. Then future papers ask if changes in the peak/notch are reliable or not. These future papers are not looking to see if there are new features in the waveform.
            – StrongBad
            Dec 10 at 16:20










          • I see - I think that explains the apparent paradox I was asking about. Thanks!
            – z8080
            Dec 10 at 16:36
















          2














          Averaging does not perfectly remove the noise. Assuming the signal is perfectly correlated across repetitions/sweeps/trials and the noise is independent, than doing from 1 to 2 sweeps will increase the SNR by 3 dB (i.e., decrease the noise by 1/2), Every doubling in the number of sweeps increases the SNR by 3 dB so that after 1024 sweeps the SNR has improved by at most 30 dB. This means that after averaging there is a minimum magnitude required for a peak/notch to be considered statistically reliable. Any ripples with smaller magnitudes are ignored since they are not statistically reliable.






          share|improve this answer





















          • This sounds to me (maybe I'm wrong) like a conflation of statistical significance and effect size. If there is also a cut-off in terms of amplitude, then why isn't it explicitly stated in papers (or is it?)
            – z8080
            Dec 10 at 16:12






          • 1




            @z8080 the first paper finds a peak/notch an assesses whether they are reliable or not. Then future papers ask if changes in the peak/notch are reliable or not. These future papers are not looking to see if there are new features in the waveform.
            – StrongBad
            Dec 10 at 16:20










          • I see - I think that explains the apparent paradox I was asking about. Thanks!
            – z8080
            Dec 10 at 16:36














          2












          2








          2






          Averaging does not perfectly remove the noise. Assuming the signal is perfectly correlated across repetitions/sweeps/trials and the noise is independent, than doing from 1 to 2 sweeps will increase the SNR by 3 dB (i.e., decrease the noise by 1/2), Every doubling in the number of sweeps increases the SNR by 3 dB so that after 1024 sweeps the SNR has improved by at most 30 dB. This means that after averaging there is a minimum magnitude required for a peak/notch to be considered statistically reliable. Any ripples with smaller magnitudes are ignored since they are not statistically reliable.






          share|improve this answer












          Averaging does not perfectly remove the noise. Assuming the signal is perfectly correlated across repetitions/sweeps/trials and the noise is independent, than doing from 1 to 2 sweeps will increase the SNR by 3 dB (i.e., decrease the noise by 1/2), Every doubling in the number of sweeps increases the SNR by 3 dB so that after 1024 sweeps the SNR has improved by at most 30 dB. This means that after averaging there is a minimum magnitude required for a peak/notch to be considered statistically reliable. Any ripples with smaller magnitudes are ignored since they are not statistically reliable.







          share|improve this answer












          share|improve this answer



          share|improve this answer










          answered Dec 10 at 15:45









          StrongBad

          1,890623




          1,890623












          • This sounds to me (maybe I'm wrong) like a conflation of statistical significance and effect size. If there is also a cut-off in terms of amplitude, then why isn't it explicitly stated in papers (or is it?)
            – z8080
            Dec 10 at 16:12






          • 1




            @z8080 the first paper finds a peak/notch an assesses whether they are reliable or not. Then future papers ask if changes in the peak/notch are reliable or not. These future papers are not looking to see if there are new features in the waveform.
            – StrongBad
            Dec 10 at 16:20










          • I see - I think that explains the apparent paradox I was asking about. Thanks!
            – z8080
            Dec 10 at 16:36


















          • This sounds to me (maybe I'm wrong) like a conflation of statistical significance and effect size. If there is also a cut-off in terms of amplitude, then why isn't it explicitly stated in papers (or is it?)
            – z8080
            Dec 10 at 16:12






          • 1




            @z8080 the first paper finds a peak/notch an assesses whether they are reliable or not. Then future papers ask if changes in the peak/notch are reliable or not. These future papers are not looking to see if there are new features in the waveform.
            – StrongBad
            Dec 10 at 16:20










          • I see - I think that explains the apparent paradox I was asking about. Thanks!
            – z8080
            Dec 10 at 16:36
















          This sounds to me (maybe I'm wrong) like a conflation of statistical significance and effect size. If there is also a cut-off in terms of amplitude, then why isn't it explicitly stated in papers (or is it?)
          – z8080
          Dec 10 at 16:12




          This sounds to me (maybe I'm wrong) like a conflation of statistical significance and effect size. If there is also a cut-off in terms of amplitude, then why isn't it explicitly stated in papers (or is it?)
          – z8080
          Dec 10 at 16:12




          1




          1




          @z8080 the first paper finds a peak/notch an assesses whether they are reliable or not. Then future papers ask if changes in the peak/notch are reliable or not. These future papers are not looking to see if there are new features in the waveform.
          – StrongBad
          Dec 10 at 16:20




          @z8080 the first paper finds a peak/notch an assesses whether they are reliable or not. Then future papers ask if changes in the peak/notch are reliable or not. These future papers are not looking to see if there are new features in the waveform.
          – StrongBad
          Dec 10 at 16:20












          I see - I think that explains the apparent paradox I was asking about. Thanks!
          – z8080
          Dec 10 at 16:36




          I see - I think that explains the apparent paradox I was asking about. Thanks!
          – z8080
          Dec 10 at 16:36


















          draft saved

          draft discarded




















































          Thanks for contributing an answer to Psychology & Neuroscience Stack Exchange!


          • Please be sure to answer the question. Provide details and share your research!

          But avoid



          • Asking for help, clarification, or responding to other answers.

          • Making statements based on opinion; back them up with references or personal experience.


          Use MathJax to format equations. MathJax reference.


          To learn more, see our tips on writing great answers.





          Some of your past answers have not been well-received, and you're in danger of being blocked from answering.


          Please pay close attention to the following guidance:


          • Please be sure to answer the question. Provide details and share your research!

          But avoid



          • Asking for help, clarification, or responding to other answers.

          • Making statements based on opinion; back them up with references or personal experience.


          To learn more, see our tips on writing great answers.




          draft saved


          draft discarded














          StackExchange.ready(
          function () {
          StackExchange.openid.initPostLogin('.new-post-login', 'https%3a%2f%2fpsychology.stackexchange.com%2fquestions%2f21283%2ferp-eeg-noise%23new-answer', 'question_page');
          }
          );

          Post as a guest















          Required, but never shown





















































          Required, but never shown














          Required, but never shown












          Required, but never shown







          Required, but never shown

































          Required, but never shown














          Required, but never shown












          Required, but never shown







          Required, but never shown







          Popular posts from this blog

          flock() on closed filehandle LOCK_FILE at /usr/bin/apt-mirror

          Mangá

           ⁒  ․,‪⁊‑⁙ ⁖, ⁇‒※‌, †,⁖‗‌⁝    ‾‸⁘,‖⁔⁣,⁂‾
”‑,‥–,‬ ,⁀‹⁋‴⁑ ‒ ,‴⁋”‼ ⁨,‷⁔„ ‰′,‐‚ ‥‡‎“‷⁃⁨⁅⁣,⁔
⁇‘⁔⁡⁏⁌⁡‿‶‏⁨ ⁣⁕⁖⁨⁩⁥‽⁀  ‴‬⁜‟ ⁃‣‧⁕‮ …‍⁨‴ ⁩,⁚⁖‫ ,‵ ⁀,‮⁝‣‣ ⁑  ⁂– ․, ‾‽ ‏⁁“⁗‸ ‾… ‹‡⁌⁎‸‘ ‡⁏⁌‪ ‵⁛ ‎⁨ ―⁦⁤⁄⁕